Village water white wild woods

Village water white wild woods

Photo by Glen Carrie on Unsplash

Originally published 29 April 2007

Here is a sen­tence from a sci­en­tif­ic report on the evo­lu­tion of lan­guage: “A chal­lenge for evo­lu­tion­ary biol­o­gy, there­fore, is to pro­vide a detailed math­e­mat­i­cal account of how nat­ur­al selec­tion can enable the emer­gence of human lan­guage from ani­mal communication.”

A love­ly, com­plex sen­tence of the kind we used to dia­gram in high school. I loved dia­gram­ming sen­tences. I loved to see a sen­tence flayed and dis­played on the page like a frog on a tray in biol­o­gy lab.

What a thing is lan­guage! Start with a bunch of nois­es — vow­els and con­so­nants — three or four dozen will do nice­ly. String them togeth­er into words and you have enough com­bi­na­tions to have an expres­sion for mil­lions of peo­ple, places, things and actions. A babe is born into the world know­ing nary a word. By age two, she will have a few hun­dred words at her com­mand. An adult might have a work­ing vocab­u­lary of tens of thou­sands of words.

Even then, we don’t go around grunt­ing words. We put them togeth­er into mean­ing­ful sen­tences using the rules of gram­mar. Sud­den­ly the num­ber of pos­si­ble utter­ances becomes essen­tial­ly infi­nite. Green Eggs and Ham is a pos­si­bil­i­ty. So is Shake­speare’s Ham­let.

Where did lan­guage come from? Chimps and gray par­rots can be taught to com­mu­ni­cate in a pared-down ver­sion of human lan­guage, but the dif­fer­ence between human speech and the most sophis­ti­cat­ed nat­ur­al ani­mal com­mu­ni­ca­tion is as dif­fer­ent as day and night.

The ter­ri­to­r­i­al calls of birds, the wig­gle dance of bees, and the mys­te­ri­ous vocal­iza­tions of whales and dol­phins are the best we get in non-human nature. Yet the lan­guage of even the most “prim­i­tive” human cul­ture is as com­plex as mod­ern Eng­lish. Clear­ly, lan­guage took a big leap for­ward as the human brain explod­ed in size and complexity.

The fact that all human lan­guages have gram­mat­i­cal sim­i­lar­i­ties sug­gests, as Chom­sky has pro­posed, an innate cor­re­spon­dence between lan­guage and the brain. But did lan­guage dri­ve brain devel­op­ment, or was it the oth­er way around? It is easy enough to under­stand the evo­lu­tion­ary pres­sures that caused our ances­tors to say “The lion is lurk­ing in the tall grass” or “There’s a nice source of flinty stones just beyond the hill,” but what dynam­ic of nat­ur­al selec­tion con­ferred upon one species among all oth­ers the abil­i­ty to say “She walks in beau­ty, like the night/ Of cloud­less climes and star­ry skies,/ And all that’s best of dark and bright/ Meet in her aspect and her eyes”?

Even when the words and gram­mar are in place, there is still some­thing called style. J. Robert Oppen­heimer, in a 1948 talk called “The Open Mind,” said that style is our way of doing jus­tice to the implic­it, the impon­der­able, and the unknown. Style is part of all com­mu­ni­ca­tion, he says, in sci­ence, pol­i­tics, lit­er­a­ture and art. “It is style which com­ple­ments affir­ma­tion with lim­i­ta­tion and humil­i­ty; it is style which makes it pos­si­ble to act effec­tive­ly, but not absolute­ly; it is style which, in the domain of for­eign pol­i­cy, enables us to find har­mo­ny between the pur­suit of ends essen­tial to us and the regard for the views, the sen­si­bil­i­ties, the aspi­ra­tions of those to whom the prob­lem may appear in anoth­er light; it is style which is the def­er­ence that action pays to uncer­tain­ty; it is above all style through which pow­er defers to reason.”

We can’t com­pile a lex­i­con of style, or dia­gram style like a sen­tence, but we know it when we see it. Evo­lu­tion­ary pres­sures may have giv­en us words for “lion” and “lurk” and “tall” and “grass,” and the rudi­ments of gram­mar, but style appears on the scene as a kind of grace — and Oppen­heimer puts his fin­ger on why it’s so impor­tant. Style tem­pers con­fi­dence with humil­i­ty, pow­er with self-restraint, sure­ness with uncer­tain­ty. Style is our escape from the inex­orable dynam­ic of evo­lu­tion, from nature red in tooth and claw.

Ama­zon now has tools on its book pages that will tell you more than you may have want­ed to know about a book. For exam­ple, thanks to Ama­zon I know that I write with an aver­age of 1.7 syl­la­bles per word and 21 words per sen­tence. Accord­ing to Ama­zon, a read­er needs about 14 years of for­mal school­ing to approach my books with under­stand­ing, and will get about 4000 words per dol­lar. I can even get a con­cor­dance of the 100 most used words in a book, in a font size pro­por­tion­al to how many times each word is used.

But none of this is style. As Oppen­heimer says, style oper­ates in the realm of the inef­fa­ble and unknown. A sci­en­tist can have style, but sci­ence can’t explain style. A preach­er can have style, but dog­ma is the ene­my of style. Style walks in beau­ty like the night. Style comes on lit­tle cat feet. Style comes hob­bling, fly­ing, run­ning, leap­ing, puff­ing and blow­ing, chuck­ling, clap­ping, crow­ing, cluck­ing and gob­bling, mop­ping and mow­ing, full of airs and graces, pulling wry faces, demure gri­maces, cat-like and rat-like, ratel and wom­bat-like, snail-paced in a hur­ry, par­rot-voiced and whistler, hel­ter-skel­ter, hur­ry-skur­ry, chat­ter­ing like mag­pies, flut­ter­ing like pigeons, glid­ing like fishes.

Style is not a chal­lenge for evo­lu­tion­ary biol­o­gy. Style is the homage we pay to a world that is mys­te­ri­ous beyond our knowing.

Share this Musing: