Scientists have ways to avoid superstition

Scientists have ways to avoid superstition

Photo by Gantas Vaičiulėnas from Pexels

Originally published 20 Jun 2000

The 18th-cen­tu­ry philoso­pher, Voltaire, wrote this about super­sti­tion: “A French­man trav­el­ing in Italy finds almost every­thing super­sti­tious, and is hard­ly wrong. The arch­bish­op of Can­ter­bury claims that the arch­bish­op of Paris is super­sti­tious; the Pres­by­te­ri­ans levy the same reproach against his Grace of Can­ter­bury, and are in their turn called super­sti­tious by the Quak­ers, who are the most super­sti­tious of men in the eyes of oth­er Christians.”

It isn’t easy to draw the bound­aries between rea­son and super­sti­tion, Voltaire observed. One per­son­’s dog­ma is anoth­er per­son­’s nonsense.

And, of course, for sci­en­tists, almost any­thing that isn’t “sci­en­tif­ic” is superstitious.

Super­sti­tion” is about the best word we have for look­ing down our noses on those who believe some­thing oth­er than what we believe ourselves.

The word comes from the Latin verb super­stare, “to stand upon or over.”

Accord­ing to the Oxford Eng­lish Dic­tio­nary, the noun form super­sti­tio prob­a­bly orig­i­nal­ly meant “stand­ing over a thing in amaze­ment or awe.”

One mod­ern mean­ing of the word — “unrea­son­ing awe or fear of some­thing unknown” — is close to the Latin root. More com­mon­ly, we use the word to mean “any irra­tional, ground­less prac­tice or belief found­ed on fear or ignorance.”

Which is about what the arch­bish­ops of Can­ter­bury and Paris thought of each oth­er in Voltaire’s time. Or, for that mat­ter, what today’s evo­lu­tion­ary biol­o­gists think of cre­ation­ists, and vice versa.

The Romans, who gave us the word, knew exact­ly what they meant by it. A super­sti­tion was any­thing strange and for­eign to the Romans. The Roman writer, Plutarch, sug­gest­ed that super­sti­tious peo­ple did not use their intel­li­gence when think­ing about the gods, which led to fanati­cism, and fanat­ics made bad cit­i­zens of the Empire.

For Romans, Chris­tian­i­ty was the super­sti­tion par excel­lence, espe­cial­ly as Chris­tians start­ed to become more numer­ous with­in the Empire. Suc­ces­sive Roman writ­ers vig­or­ous­ly con­demned the super­sti­tious beliefs of Christians.

Of course, when Con­stan­tine led the Empire toward Chris­tian­i­ty in the fourth cen­tu­ry, sud­den­ly super­sti­tion was on the oth­er foot.

After Con­stan­tine, the Greek and Roman gods — Diony­sus, Athena, Jupiter, Mars, and all the Olympian pan­theon — became the new super­sti­tion, and it was as super­sti­tion that we learned the Gre­co-Roman myths in school. In my school, we also learned of the “super­sti­tions” of Hin­dus, Moslems, Native Amer­i­cans, and all the rest.

In these more ecu­meni­cal times, we are less inclined to dis­miss reli­gious faiths oth­er than our own as super­sti­tious. We tend to reserve the “s” word for such things as black cats bring­ing bad luck, or, if we are sci­en­tif­i­cal­ly inclined, for astrol­o­gy, para­psy­chol­o­gy, home­opa­thy, and a host of oth­er “irra­tional” beliefs.

But, if these things are “irra­tional,” as most sci­en­tists (includ­ing me) pious­ly believe, then why do so many peo­ple believe them to be true?

Even in Roman times, the physi­cian Galen warned his stu­dents and col­leagues how easy it is to believe mere­ly because we inher­it the reli­gious or philo­soph­i­cal beliefs of our par­ents, teach­ers, or native city. But we also have a great capac­i­ty to deceive ourselves.

Back in the 1940s, the behav­ioral psy­chol­o­gist B. F. Skin­ner did a famous exper­i­ment with pigeons that he thought had some rel­e­vance to human super­sti­tions. He put birds in the kind of cages used for train­ing ani­mals by rein­force­ment — peck a bar, get some bird­seed, that sort of thing. Except in this new exper­i­ment, the feed was pro­vid­ed at reg­u­lar inter­vals regard­less of what the pigeons did.

And guess what? The pigeons fell into cer­tain behav­iors all by them­selves — nod­ding or turn­ing or peck­ing for food — although their behav­iors had noth­ing to do with the reward being offered.

Skin­ner wrote: “A few acci­den­tal con­nec­tions between a rit­u­al and favor­able con­se­quences suf­fice to set up and main­tain the behav­ior in spite of many unre­in­forced instances.”

The exper­i­ment might be said to demon­strate a sort of super­sti­tion. The bird behaves as if there were a casu­al rela­tion between its behav­ior and the pre­sen­ta­tion of food, although such a rela­tion is lack­ing. There are many analo­gies to human behavior.”

Do we con­vince our­selves that astrol­o­gy, para­psy­chol­o­gy, or home­opa­thy “work” because they pro­vide a kind of psy­cho­log­i­cal reward, as Skin­ner implies? And, if so, how do sci­en­tists avoid falling into the trap of superstition?

Answer: By rely­ing upon dou­ble-blind exper­i­ments, place­bos, repro­ducibil­i­ty, peer review, unre­lent­ing skep­ti­cism, and all the rest of the tricks-of-the-trade we call the sci­en­tif­ic method.

It’s not per­fect. Even as we hold our sci­en­tif­ic the­o­ries to the refin­ing fire of expe­ri­ence, we should remem­ber the arch­bish­ops of Can­ter­bury and Paris, and know that humans, like pigeons, may be wired for self-deception.

Share this Musing: