Instinctive speech diminishes us not

Instinctive speech diminishes us not

Photo by Stephen Andrews on Unsplash

Originally published 6 November 2001

Is lan­guage some­thing we are born with, or is it some­thing we learn in the first few years of life?

The answer would appear to be obvi­ous. No babe comes talk­ing out of the womb. Every child must learn a vocab­u­lary, learn to match sounds to things or concepts.

Yet there are lin­guists who say that some aspects of lan­guage are inborn. The per­son most often asso­ci­at­ed with this view is Noam Chom­sky, who, in a series of bril­liant works in the sec­ond half of the last cen­tu­ry, pro­posed that all human lan­guages share a Uni­ver­sal Gram­mar that cor­re­sponds to innate struc­tures of the human brain.

Chom­sky was not the first to hold this view — even Dar­win guessed as much — but he was the first to present a com­pelling argu­ment based on com­mon fea­tures of all lan­guages and on obser­va­tions of lan­guage acqui­si­tion by children.

No one doubts that genes deter­mine our acqui­si­tion of arms and legs, and few doubt that our ten­den­cy to smile when hap­py and frown when sad are instinc­tive. But the idea that lan­guage is part­ly pro­grammed by genes gen­er­ates a sur­pris­ing amount of controversy.

We just don’t like to think that some­thing so fun­da­men­tal­ly human as speech can be part of our ani­mal nature. Lan­guage is some­thing we share with the angels, not the beasts — or so we like to think.

And when you think about it, the idea that a Uni­ver­sal Gram­mar is inborn does seem absurd.

After all, what we inher­it from our par­ents is an arm’s length of DNA, tan­gled on 23 chro­mo­somes. The human genome is a sequence of four mol­e­cules, dubbed A, T, C, and G by biol­o­gists. The code of life is writ­ten with these four chem­i­cal “let­ters,” three bil­lion let­ters in all, com­pris­ing about 30,000 genes.

The DNA spins off pro­teins, sequences of 22 amino acids that deter­mine the shape of the pro­tein. How pro­teins act in the body is deter­mined by their shapes and chem­i­cal affinities.

What Chom­sky and his fol­low­ers ask us to believe is that uni­ver­sal rules of gram­mar some­how reside in the pro­tein struc­ture of the brain. When a child says “Shoe off” or “We hold­ed the kit­ty,” it is not an act of imi­ta­tion — for sure­ly no adult says such things — but an inborn ten­den­cy to put words togeth­er in cer­tain ways. Chem­i­cals, just chemicals.

And yet, as strange as Chom­sky’s idea seems, I would­n’t bet against it. The organ­ic mol­e­cules of life are amaz­ing­ly ver­sa­tile, and some things that chem­i­cals obvi­ous­ly encode — such as the unguid­ed flight of New Eng­land’s monarch but­ter­flies to a tiny patch of Mex­i­can for­est they have nev­er been to before — are scarce­ly less won­der­ful than speech.

Now that the human genome has been sequenced, biol­o­gists can start look­ing for the puta­tive lan­guage genes, those seg­ments of our DNA that are crit­i­cal to the nor­mal acqui­si­tion of lan­guage. The first iden­ti­fi­ca­tion of a “lan­guage gene” recent­ly has been report­ed by British researchers. It is called the FOXP2 gene on chro­mo­some 7, and seems to play a role in devel­op­ment of the brain cir­cuit­ry that under­lies speech. Its dis­rup­tion leads to an inher­it­ed speech impairment.

Writ­ing in Nature, lin­guist Steven Pinker stat­ed: “The dis­cov­ery of a gene impli­cat­ed in speech and lan­guage is among the first fruits of the Human Genome Project for the cog­ni­tive sci­ences. Just as the 1990s are remem­bered as the decade of the brain and the dawn of cog­ni­tive neu­ro­science, the first decade of the 21st cen­tu­ry may well be thought of as the decade of the gene and the dawn of cog­ni­tive genetics.”

But, in spite of the iden­ti­fi­ca­tion of a lan­guage gene, the idea that speech is instinc­tive will con­tin­ue to be fierce­ly resist­ed. Many peo­ple, includ­ing some schol­ars and sci­en­tists, hold that an essen­tial part of our human nature, includ­ing the abil­i­ty to express ideas in speech, is inde­pen­dent of our ani­mal bod­ies, an imma­te­r­i­al “ghost in the machine” that will sur­vive our chem­i­cal dis­so­lu­tion. My guess is that sci­en­tif­ic research in the com­ing cen­tu­ry will offer no sup­port for this ancient view.

Instead, we will dis­cov­er that not only lan­guage but also con­scious­ness and self-aware­ness are firm­ly anchored in our bio­chem­i­cal natures, inex­tri­ca­bly tied to the cir­cuit­ry of brains that are woven on the loom of DNA. None of this, it seems to me, dimin­ish­es our esti­ma­tion of the human self; rather, it vast­ly expands our appre­ci­a­tion for the com­plex­i­ty and beau­ty of a crea­ture who can say “We hold­ed the kit­ty” or — with the 17th-cen­tu­ry poet John Donne—“Death be not proud, though some have called thee mighty and dread­ful, for thou art not so.”

In anoth­er poem, Donne wrote, “I am a lit­tle world made cun­ning­ly of ele­ments and an angel­ic sprite.” It now appears that our angel­ic sprite­ness is made of ele­ments, too.

Share this Musing: