Birds of a different feather

Birds of a different feather

Rufous Fantail feeding Brush Cuckoo • Photo by Damien.cook.frog (CC BY-SA 4.0)

Originally published 18 February 1991

Con­sid­er the cuckoo.

When the female cuck­oo is ready to lay, she seeks a bird of anoth­er species that is build­ing a nest — a reed war­bler, say. The cuck­oo perch­es on a near­by branch and waits. When the nest is fin­ished, the reed war­bler lays a clutch of eggs, at dawn. Some­time dur­ing ear­ly after­noon, when the male and female war­blers are away for­ag­ing for food, the cuck­oo glides onto the nest and lays one egg, iden­ti­cal in col­or and pat­tern to the reed war­bler eggs, though some­what larg­er. She takes one war­bler egg in her beak and flies away, the whole oper­a­tion hav­ing tak­en less than 10 seconds.

The cuck­oo eats the stolen egg for lunch.

The war­blers return to the nest and sit on the eggs, until the cuck­oo egg, which incu­bates quick­est, hatch­es. Out pops a chick, and the first thing it does, even before its eyes are open, is toss the oth­er eggs — the authen­tic reed war­bler eggs — out of the nest.

The war­blers are per­haps a bit baf­fled by the miss­ing eggs, but nev­er­the­less they rear the changeling chick, think­ing it their own. Soon the cuck­oo has grown much larg­er (and eight times heav­ier!) than its tiny sur­ro­gate par­ents. The war­blers con­tin­ue to stuff the cuck­oo’s gap­ing mouth with bugs and caterpillars.

It is nature’s ulti­mate scam, and for the cuck­oo the last word in low-cost, no-both­er parenting.

Britain’s bamboozled birds

In Britain, where the cuck­oo’s call is a much-loved fea­ture of the land­scape, four species of birds are vic­tim­ized — reed war­blers, mead­ow pip­its, dun­nocks, and pied wag­tails, all of them much small­er than the cuck­oo. There seem to be four genet­i­cal­ly dis­tinct strains of female cuck­oos, each spe­cial­iz­ing in one host species. Except for the cuck­oo that lays her egg in the dun­nock­’s nest, each female’s egg close­ly resem­bles the eggs of the select­ed host.

Enter, the cuckoo-ologists.

Nicholas Davies and Michael Brooke are two Cam­bridge Uni­ver­si­ty ornithol­o­gists who spe­cial­ize in cuck­oos. They have described their work in the Jan­u­ary [1991] issue of Sci­en­tif­ic Amer­i­can. You’d have to be a lit­tle cuck­oo to do what these guys do, but their research ele­gant­ly demon­strates the pow­er of evo­lu­tion­ary the­o­ry to explain nat­ur­al curiosities.

And the behav­iors of cuck­oos and their bam­boo­zled hosts offer nat­ur­al curiosi­ties aplenty.

Davies and Brooke armed them­selves with pho­ny cuck­oo eggs, made of resin, the exact size and weight of real cuck­oo eggs, and paint­ed to resem­ble the dif­fer­ent eggs laid by the four strains of female cuck­oos. Then they played cuck­oo. They snitched real eggs from reed war­bler nests and replaced them with pho­ny cuck­oo eggs. The war­blers accept­ed eggs that resem­bled their own and reject­ed most of the oth­ers, push­ing them out of the nests.

Clear­ly, reed war­blers aren’t with­out some pow­ers of dis­crim­i­na­tion, and nat­ur­al selec­tion would favor a cuck­oo egg that close­ly resem­bles the host’s. The cuck­oo eggs evolved accord­ing­ly, a clas­sic exam­ple of mim­ic­ry in nature.

Surreptitious switches

This was just the begin­ning. Davies and Brooke sys­tem­at­i­cal­ly replaced eggs in the nests of all four species of host birds, with pho­ny eggs of every type, at dif­fer­ent times of the day, remov­ing dif­fer­ent num­bers of host eggs, and every oth­er com­bi­na­tion of thiev­ing and con­found­ing they could think of. They even went to Ice­land to try their sur­rep­ti­tious switch­es on mead­ow pip­its and wag­tails that have long lived in iso­la­tion from cuckoos.

Every response of the cuck­oos and their hosts to pho­ny eggs was con­sis­tent with nat­ur­al selec­tion. For exam­ple, Ice­landic birds were more eas­i­ly fooled by pho­ny eggs than their British cousins; they have not need­ed to evolve defens­es against cuck­oo trick­ery. And the cuck­oo that lays its eggs in the dun­nock­’s nest has no need of egg mim­ic­ry; the dun­nock accepts almost any egg as its own, regard­less of col­or or pat­tern, per­haps because it has only recent­ly been par­a­sitized by cuckoos.

What we have here appears to be a true case of co-evo­lu­tion: Cuck­oos have respond­ed to the host’s defens­es by evolv­ing eggs that close­ly resem­ble host eggs. Hosts, in turn, have adapt­ed to cuck­oo par­a­sitism by becom­ing ever more dis­crim­i­nat­ing and less like­ly to be fooled.

Accord­ing to Davies and Brooke, the result of all this knav­ish behav­ior on the part of cuck­oos has been an evo­lu­tion­ary “arm’s race” between cuck­oos and their hosts, lead­ing to ever more intri­cate adap­tions and counter-adaptations.

The most delight­ful thing of all about this sto­ry is the thought of the two cuck­oo-olo­gists, their pock­ets full of pho­ny cuck­oo eggs, skulk­ing around in marsh and moor try­ing to unrav­el the ways of evo­lu­tion. Such behav­ior on the part of humans is sure­ly a nat­ur­al curios­i­ty as wor­thy of inves­ti­ga­tion as the egg-lay­ing habits of cuck­oos, but pos­si­bly rather more resis­tant to expla­na­tion by nat­ur­al selection.

Share this Musing: