Did Darwin make him do it?

Did Darwin make him do it?

“David Garrick Between Tragedy and Comedy” by Joshua Reynolds (1761)

Originally published 12 October 1998

Birds do it, bees do it, even edu­cat­ed fleas do it.

What do they do?

With apolo­gies to Cole Porter, they cheat on their mates.

Repub­li­cans do it, preach­ers do it, even — oh dear! — teach­ers do it.

Which is why the Amer­i­can peo­ple seem to have decid­ed that Bill Clin­ton should not be impeached.

Extra­mar­i­tal han­ky-panky may be tacky. It may be sleazy. It may even be deeply immoral and — in the event of per­jury — ille­gal. But it is not a high crime or mis­de­meanor, at least accord­ing to the folks who respond to polls.

So what does this mean? Is Amer­i­ca in a moral decline, more tol­er­ant of adul­tery than in the past? Or do we instinc­tive­ly know that humans — males espe­cial­ly — are bio­log­i­cal­ly inclined to fool around, and that monogamy is a cul­tur­al or reli­gious ide­al at odds with our genes?

Bill betrayed his spouse and embar­rassed the nation, but he may have been fol­low­ing a script writ­ten in his DNA rather than in his moral con­science. In the old days, the excuse was “the dev­il made me do it.” Nowa­days, we are like­ly to ascribe our unwor­thy instincts to our Dar­win­ian legacy.

Nat­ur­al selec­tion, it would seem, favors the male who spreads his genes by father­ing as many off­spring as pos­si­ble, and favors the female who picks the best pos­si­ble genes for her off­spring. Advan­tages can accrue to both males and females who cheat on a per­ma­nent mate, and cer­tain­ly, in the ani­mal king­dom, fideli­ty is the excep­tion rather than the rule.

A sur­vey of recent research in the jour­nal Sci­ence sug­gests that only about 10 per­cent of appar­ent­ly monog­a­mous birds and mam­mals are faith­ful to their part­ners. Even among those tra­di­tion­al paragons of mar­i­tal virtue, the blue­birds, it turns out that females slip away for brief flings with oth­er males.

Among our clos­est ani­mal kin, the pri­mates, infi­deli­ty is the rule. Males tend to look for liaisons with lots of younger, nubile part­ners. Females are gen­er­al­ly less promis­cu­ous, but when they do have affairs they go for part­ners with pow­er, access to resources, or pres­tige. Only females already paired with pow­er­ful males tend to be faithful.

With adul­tery so preva­lent among our pri­mate cousins, it would be sur­pris­ing if some­thing of the wan­der­ing eye were not part of our own evo­lu­tion­ary inheritance.

Assess­ing the eth­no­log­i­cal evi­dence avail­able in 1993, sci­ence jour­nal­ist Matt Rid­ley wrote: “The nature of the human male, then, is to take oppor­tu­ni­ties, if they are grant­ed him, for polyg­a­mous mat­ing and to use wealth, pow­er, and vio­lence as means to sex­u­al ends in the com­pe­ti­tion with oth­er men — though usu­al­ly not at the expense of sac­ri­fic­ing a secure monog­a­mous relationship.”

If nat­ur­al selec­tion favors adul­tery, and if it is the nature of human males (and some­times females) to cheat, then where did our monog­a­mous ide­al come from?

More to the point, why has the ide­al of fideli­ty to a per­ma­nent mate sur­vived even in those human cul­tures where polygamy is sanc­tioned and adul­tery rampant?

This is one of those ques­tions that evo­lu­tion­ary biol­o­gists love to answer. Most make a case for a max­i­mum selec­tive advan­tage for those indi­vid­u­als who com­bine a sta­ble part­ner­ship with a lit­tle some­thing on the side. Such indi­vid­u­als get the best of both worlds — genet­i­cal­ly speaking.

Of course, all such expla­na­tions have some­thing of the “just-so sto­ry” about them. The truth is, no one real­ly knows if our human pat­terns of part­ner­ing are genet­i­cal­ly or cul­tur­al­ly deter­mined — or some mix of both.

But we can learn a few things from study­ing oth­er ani­mals: Polygamy is most wide­spread among species with a high degree of sex­u­al dimor­phism; that is, the male is con­sid­er­ably larg­er than the female. Among such species, a few pow­er­ful males tend to monop­o­lize poten­tial mates, and less pow­er­ful males do with­out or sneak what they can.

Com­pe­ti­tion among males of polyg­a­mous species is intense. When a younger, stronger male over­throws the lord of a harem, he will often mur­der the young off­spring of the deposed patri­arch, there­by remov­ing com­pe­ti­tion for his own genes.

Like­wise, among humans, polygamy has flour­ished pri­mar­i­ly with­in despot­ic, vio­lent soci­eties. King Solomon, it is said, had a thou­sand con­cu­bines, which meant there were a thou­sand less pow­er­ful fel­lows who could only look on with envy.

I like to think that our monog­a­mous ide­al springs not from our genes but from the same cul­tur­al ten­den­cies as Jef­fer­son­ian democ­ra­cy and the Ser­mon on the Mount: The notion that in the best soci­ety, every­one — not just the most pow­er­ful males and most nubile females — has equal access to life, lib­er­ty and the pur­suit of mates.

We can under­stand Bill Clin­ton, but not be proud of him. His behav­ior may be typ­i­cal of our species, and cer­tain­ly of birds and mam­mals, but he is not an exem­plar of our high­est aspi­ra­tions to indi­vid­ual equal­i­ty and the respon­si­bil­i­ties of love.

Our abil­i­ty to opt for some­thing bet­ter than our brute natures is what makes us unique­ly human.

Share this Musing: